AESTRACT
The common cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis, is able to exhibit
a wide yariety of body patterns. Fast studies have analyzed the
animal's ability to camouflage itself using subjective methods.
This study encorporated two objective methods, overall tone
analysis and spatial frequency of pattern analysis, to examine
how well the animal was matching its background. Un its natural
background, sand, the cuttlefish matched both tone and pattern.
On an unnatural background, such as a uniform gray, one animal
matched tone, ame matched pattern and one attempted to match both
tane and pattern. On analysis combining subjective method and
the quantitative methods described in this paper offer a more
definitive method for judging the degree to which the the animal
can match the surrounding environment in behavioral tests.
INTEODUCTICN
The cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis, has fascinated biologists
because of its ability to change its skin patterns within
fractians of a zecond. Using the brown, yellow and red pigmented
ordans in their skin (chromatophores), which are under neural
control, the cuttlefish are able to exhibit a wide variety of
hody pattemns. For example, cuttlefish use patterns to scare
prey, to attract mates, and to camoflauge themselves. Early
studies, by Bott (1938) and by Tinbergen (1939) concentrated on
the cuttlefish's courtship patterns and Holmes (1940) described
various other patterns. More recently, Hanlon et. al (1986)
systematically characterised the elements and units that go to
make up the body patterns. Hanlon divided the body patterns into
a total of 34 chromatic components, 16 light and i8 dark and, in
addition, described sis textural components, 8 postural
components, and o locomator components. Textural components
reter to the texture of the animal and the dermal papillae that
can expand or contract to cause a rough or emooth surface.
Postural components refer to the way the animal is positioned.
and locomotor components to the motion of the animal. Althouch
the animal can exhibit these components in almost any
combination, Hanlon characterized 13 whole body patterns. Cott
(1940) outlined the ways in which animals can conceal themselves
from predators: colour resemblance, deceptive resemblance,
obliterative shading, disruptive colouration, coincident
disruptive colpuration and concealment. Hanlon (1986) describes
how cuttlefish employ all the techniques of colour matching set
out by Catt.
Hanlon's study describes how the cuttlefish match in a
descriptive, qualitative way; and he noted that his attempts to
quanitty matching were unsatisfactory. The purpose of this study
was to tind a more quantative, gjective way of examining the
mechaniem by which Sepia officinalis match their background
utilising recent advances in the field of digital imag
processing. Computer analysis can be used to obtain numerical
data in an accurate, rapid and reproducable manner (lnoue, 1786).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Sepia were hatched in Galveston, Texas and brought to
Hopkins Marine Station, Facific Grove, Ca. at around A menthe ot
age. The animals used were between four and five monthe old at
the time of testing. Typical mantle length of the animals was a
cm. The three animals used for the study were kept in shaded,
individual aquaria with running sea water  15" C. Fictures of
individual animals were taken on three different backgrounds: (1)
amall grained sand substrate, (E) a uniform light gray background
and (S) a mock mubstrate cantaining rocks approkimately the mame
zite as the animal, but in a variety of tomes. In an experimental
tank, the test animal was placed in a 17 cm bowl containing of
gne of the three substrates and given five minutes to aclimate.
Fictures of the cuttlefish were taken with a m Wikon camera
and twa electric flashes held on stands om sither side ot the
tank. Ektachrome 100 alide film was used. The camera was tastened
th a trippd approkimately mne meter above the tank. Since evem
slight movement often seemed to bother the animals, the pictures
were taken in a darkemed room and I stayed sitting quietly beside
the tank fom the five minutes before the pictures were taken.
In order to have some way of subjectively judging how well
the cuttlefish matched their background, the pictures of the
cuttlefish in the experimental tank were shown to 12 human
judges' who were asked to rate the cuttlefish's camoutlage
performance on a scale of O (a bad match) to 3 (a good match).
Eody patterns of the animals were classified according to
the system develcped by Hanlon (1984). The five patterne shown
by the animals were:
(1) Uniform light (see figure 2): to an overall light tone with
very few of no dark chromatophores enpanded.
(2) Stipple (see figure 4): an overall uniform tone that is
slightly darker than l due to expansion of dark chromatophor.
(3) Weak dismuptive (see figure 6): a wide variety of patterns,
with the common characteristic that the body is braken into
portions by transverse or lengitudinal elements.
(4) Weak Zebra (see figure 5): a low contrast striped patterm
also calledsandy-tripe' by Holmes (1740).
() Uniform Dark (see figure 3): expansion of all dark
chromatophores.
To objectively measure the animals ability to match its
hackground, the various slides were then digitized using a
MegaVision 1OE4-XM computer and a Panasonic black and white vided
camera, By selecting the function 'tyscannem' the colour slides
T y  array of pixels with
werestored in the computer in a
mach pixel assigned a value on a gray scale of 25 ( O being the
blatkesta-
eing the whitest). The gray scaleof the
Hegalision was fpund to be linear at all but the darkest and
whitest gray values (see figure 7). The Megavision function
'hpolygon' was used to output gray values within the area covered
by the cuttlefish, and an equal size area on the adjacent
substrate. The function 'hlist' was used to obtain the mean,
standard deviation, miniumum and maximum values, skew and
kurtosis of the histograms of the pixel gray scales from
cuttlefish and background. Each histogram quantatively measures
how well the animal was matches the oyerall tone of the
zubstrate.
A spectral analysis methed was used to determine how well the
cuttlefish were matching pattern. Spectral analysis is oftem
used to interpret time in terms of frequency (Chatfield 1784).
Im this study, it was used to comvert distance in space (on the
hack of the cuttlefish or on the substrate) into spatial
trequency. The result of the analysis is presented as a
speatrum. The abtissa represents the spatial frequency of
periodic pattemns in inverme mm. and the ordinate is the power
spentrum, a measure af the variance in gray scale associated with
mach spatial frequency (Chatfield, 1984).
The function 'profile' was used to draw a transect 9
pixels wide acros the back of the animal and an equal-length
transect through the adjacent substrate. Each pixel corresponds
to a distance in space of .Emm. The gray values obtained were
then transferred tw an IEM XI personal computer. Here, the gray
meale values were filtemed with a high pass digital filtem to
remave the mean and the first five harmonics of the fundamental
spatial frequengy. The filtering was done ta memoye the low
trequency patterns associated with the averall length of the
transect on the animal or substrate. Next, the power spectrum of
a Tho pixel series was calculated and smoothed using a nine-point
running average, and the objective measure of pattern matching
was obtained.
RESULTS
Histogram pairs of gray scale (a measure of overall tome).
ame shown in figures 8-13 for each test cuttlefish and its
background. A student t-test was performed on the means of each
pair. Due to the extremely large number of pixels in each
histmgram (n240,000), even a difference in means of one was
statistically significant. However, the human eye can only
distinguish approximately Jo shades of gray (Takahashi, 1986).
Theretere, the human eye can discern a difference in light
intensity only if two regions of the image are more than 7
(2w6/30) apart on the gray scale. The means of the histograms,
them, were comsidered 'different' if the means were farther than
apart.
Un the sand substrate, the difference in means for all three
animals was less than sevem. On the gray substrate, all animals
were greater than 7 gray scale units away from the background
mean. The histogram of the cuttlefish on the rock background was
compared to three other histegrams: that of one rock in the bowl.
the whole area of the bowl and a cuttlefish sised area within the
bowl. The cuttlefish judged the most difficult to see matched
(difference in means 47) a rock in the bowl, but not overall tome
or a cuttlefish-sied area in the bowl. Conversely, the mean of
the histogram of the animal yoted most visible did not match any
of the means.
The results of the spectral analysis are shown in tigures
14-17. The cuttlefish and its background are plotted on thesame
zet of axes. All the spectra exhibit a large peak corresponding
to a spacing of 23.T mm. This peak was theught to be an artifact
uf the filtering process and was therefore not included in the
analysis. The cuttlefish which closely match their substrate
according to the zubjective judging, show that the pattern
spacings are within .omm, at most. Those cuttlefish judged nat
to be matching have harmonics that differ by more than twoee
animal +).
IhE power spectra also compare the variance of the transect
on the cuttlefish with that of the background. Cuttletisn
photographed on sand show less variance than the substrate. The
animals phatographed on the gray background show greater variance
than the suhstrate, while those compared to the rack hackground
mhow mither more or less variance than the mock aubstrate. he
variance exhibited on the mocks does nat ampear to be asmociated
with an individual animal or the trial, as each individual showed
greater or leszer variance at different times.
Ihe resulte of the individual animals body pattern choite is
aummari zed in the bar graphs om hoth rack and gray substrates.
All the animals except for animal  n the gray background use
more than one pattern when confronted with the same substrate.
Un both the rock and gray substrates, five different body
patterns were used. Similar bar graphs for the sand trials were
not made, since all the animals matched the sand substrate well.
and all showed the same overall body pattern.
DISCUSSION
Cuttletish on the sand substrate are able to match both tone
and pattern and are subjectively well camouflaged. Since sand is
the natural habitat of the animal it seems logical that all the
animals would be able to match the sand backorpund best. When
contronted with an unnatural substrate, rocks or gray, the
cuttletish use various strategies to conceal themselyes. On the
gray background, animal  matched the uniform pattern well, but
was highly visible because of lack of tone matching, (see figure
3) Animal  matched the overall tone of the gray backargund to
within one shade of gray, but displayed a pattern that was highly
visible. (see figure 4) Animal  matched tone to within 5 shades
as well as matching pattern.
he results on the rock substrate also indicate that
different strategies can be used by the same individual.
Individuals employ at least five different body patterns to
mamoutlaque themselves on the unnatural substratee, Thus, when
placed on an unfamiliar or unnatural background, the cuttlefish
have the ability to respond in a variety of ways. The animal is
able to examine his environment and then able to chosse the
pattern and tome it will show. Evidently, the neural circuitry
involved in chromatophore response is not "hard-wired".
Similarly, the same cuttlefish does not always respond the
same way to the same background (see figure 20). With the
exception of S on the gray substrate, each of the three animals
exhibited at least two different body patterns when placed on the
same substrate repeatedly. Moreover, the patterns did not depend
on how many times the cuttlefish had seen the particular
zubstrate before.
Ihis study demonstrates a method for quantifying matching.
Uverall tone of the animal can be quantified using a digital
image processor and pattern matching can be cbjectively
determined by the power spectra. This objective, quantative
method is especially powerful for judging matching on uniform
mubstrates such am sand or the light gray background. Subjective
analysis is still necessany, especially on a complex substrate
(like a rock background), where it is not always evident how the
amimal is trying to camouflage itself. Subjective analysis and
the two cbjective methods outlined above, together provide a good
basis for judging how well, and how, cuttlefish match their
sumstrate, and the different strategies they use.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I would like to thank Mark Denny and Stuart Thompson for all
their advice, help and most of all, patience, throughout this
quarter. Thanks also to Chris Fatton for all his help in
photography as well as Steve Gaines and Mark Shibata for their
computer help. Hoger Hanlon deserves many thanks for supplying
the cuttlefish, many papers (including his own unpublished work)
and for teaching all of us in the Spring Class in an entertaining
and informative way. Finally, thanks to everyone in Bio 17SH
Hopkins Spring Class for making this quarter nifty!
ished.
HEFERENCES
Berns, M. and Walter, K. 1986 Digital image processing and
(ed. Shinya ounep
analysis. In Video Microscopy.
Hew York: Flenum Fress, 584 pages.
In Cephalopod life
Boletzky, S.V.
1983 Sepia officinalis.
Eyeles. Volume 1: species accounts.
(ed. F.R. Boyle), pp. 1-
London: Academic Fress, 475 pages.
Chatfield, C. 1940 The analysis of a time semies: an Introduction
(End ed) London: Chapman and Hall, 28 pages.
Cott, H.B. 1940 Adaptive coloration in animals. London:
Metheun amp Co. Ltd.
Hanlon, R.T. and Hessenger, J.B. (1786) Adaptive coloration in
young cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis L.): The morphology and
develmpment of body patterns and their relation to behavior. Unp
Holmes, W. 1940 the colour changes and colour patterns of Sepia
mfficinalis L. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. A110, 17-25.
Takahashi, 1785 Colom science (ed. B. Styles) End ed.
FIGURE LEGEND
Figure 1: Animalon sand background
Figure 2: Animal  on gray background
Figure : Onimal on gray background
Finure 4: Animalon gray backgrounc
Figure 5: Animalon rock background
Figure a: Onimal  on rock background
Figure 7: Gray scale versus relative intensity plot trom
Megavision computer illustrating the linear gray scale.
Fioure 8: Top: Histogram plot of Animal  on sand measuring gray
scale value and number of pixels. Bottom: Histogram of sand
substrate. see tet.
Figure 9: Histograms of Animal i on gray and gray substrate
Figure 10: Histogram of Animal  on gray and gray substrate
Figure i1: Histogram of Onimal  on gray and gray substrate
Figure 12: Top Right: Histogram of animal l on racks. Top lett:
histogram of large, square rock in the background. Bottom right:
histogram of an area in the bowl the same size as the animal.
bottom left: histogram output of whole bowl containing rocks.
Fidure 13: Histograms of Onimal  on rocks and various parts of
the substrate.
Finure 14: Fower spectral density function of animal i on sand.
FEARE correspond to the distance between repeating gray values im
pace.
Figure I: Fower spectral density function of Animal i on gray
zubstrate.
Figure 16: Fower spectral density function of Animal on gray
zubstrate.
Figure 17: Fower spectral density function of Animal  on gray
zubstrate.
Figure 18: Fower spectral density function of Animal l on rock
bachargund. The graphs correspond to the animal's back and to
one reck in the substrate (the large square one). he graphe
corresponding to the area nest to the animal or the whole bowl
nat shown.
Figure 19: see explanation for figure 18. Fower spectral density
function of Onimal .
Figure 20: Top: comparison of the various body patterns shown on
aray substrate by animals 1,2 and 3. Bottom: comparison of body
patterns exhibited on the rock substrate. Both graphs illustrate
the yaristy of patterns that are used by the animal to camoutlage
itself.
Fioure Ei: the percentages of patterns shown on the rock
substrate and on the gray substrate.
FIGURE
FIGURE
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 6
C
—
—


—
1





(
FIGURE 7
atata-




ktaa-
+
FIGURE 8
ANIMAL 3 ON SAND
1.0-
75
50
25

.00-
191
63
12.
GRAY UQLUE
1.O-SAND
.75
.50-
25


O0
191
12.
63
GRAY UQLUE
MEAN 1SS
STANDARD DEUIATION 22
255
MEAN 183
SIANDARD DEUIATION 23
255
FIGURE 9
OANIMALON GRAY
75
50-
25
MEAN 148
STANDARD DEUIATION 25
L
.00
191 255
63
GRAY UQLUE
1.0- GRAY
75-
50-
25-
MEAN 172
STANDARD DEUIATION S
00-
255
191
63
12
GRAY UQLUE
FIGURE 10
1.0 AMMAL 42 ON GRAV
75-
50
2

O0
3
12
191
GRAT UQLUE
1.0— GRAY
75
50-
2
00
63
12
191
GRAY UALUE
255
MEAN ES
STANDARD DEUIATION 43
MEAN 13
SIANDARD DEUIATION 2
55
FIGURE 11
1 O-ANIMAL 43 ON GRAV
50
25
MEAN 14
STANDAFO DEVIATION 22

00
191 255
127
63
GRAY UALUE
GRAY
1.0-
75-
50
25
MEAN
STANDARD DEUIATION

O0
191 255
63
127
GRAT UQLUE
FIGURE 12
ANIMAL ON ROCKS
90
MEAN 143
SIANDARD DEUIATION 2e
5
50-
25-

OO
255
191
127
63
GRAY UALUE
.0 WHOLE
BOWL
MEAN 127
SIANDARD DEUIATION S
25
50-
25

OU
191
255
63
GRAY UQLUE
ONE ROCK
MEAN 84
STANDARD DEUIATION IE
H.25
50-
25-

00
191
127
63
GRAY UALUE
1.0- AREA NEXT TO ANIMAL
MEAN 112
STANDARD DEUIATION S3
H25
50-
25

00
191
127
63
GRAY UQLUE
FIGURE 13
ANIMAL *3 ON ROCKS
1.0
MEAN S7
SIANDAPC DEUIATICN 2
5
8.50-
25-

00
63
12.
191
GRAY UQLUE
1.0 AREAI NEXT TO ANIMAL
MEAN 7
SIANDARD DEVIATION 34
75
.50-
25-

00
63
12.
191
255
GRAY UQLUE
255
1.O
H75
50-
25-
00-
1.O-
H.75
50-
25-
00
ONE
ROCK
MEAN
STANDARD DEVIATION 15

63
127
191 2
GRAY UQLUE
WHOLE BOWL
MEAN 12
STANDARD DEUIATION as
L
191 2
63
GROY UQLUE
N
J
I
+
— ——
—








+

J


(sousnou)
Whaas o

FIGURE 14
Z
J
FIGURE 15



Whaltat am

8


—

—
Q
Z
I
FIGURE 16







paaa-
J
N
—
I
FIGURE 17



—

snou
J


staa-



2

8
wha a



FIGURE 18
0
N
J
FIGURE 19


——







waa
—
FICURE 20 PATTEFMIMIE OM GFAY SUESTEATE
100 +
90 -
80 -
60 -
50
40
50



l
ER
Animal +2
Animal #3
Animal fl
EMING ON ROCK SUBSTRATE
A
100
90 -
30 -
70
50
50
40
30 -
L


t
Animal 13
Animal 2
NVEAK TEBRA SN DAR
VSTPPE QVEAK OISAUPTNE N LIOET
FIGURE 21
PAUIERNING ON ROCRS AND GEAT
100 -
90
80 -
70 -
50 -
40 -
30

20



101
4


Et Lt
oEl
GRA
ROCKS
NWEAK TBRA
sroote
JWEAK DISRUPTIVE HAUN LIGHT
QUN. DARK