ABSTRACT Chemical cues from macrocystis and mysid are capable of triggering behaviors in the nudibranch mollusc, Melibe leonina The behaviors ellicited are appropriate to the stimulus. (Le. Melibe locomote in response to macrocystis, and exhibit feeding behavior in response to mysid.) These behaviors are not seen when the animals are in an isolated environment or are stimulated solely by touch or vision. Furthermore, Melibe exhibit the ability to integrate chemical cues. while neither brine shrimp nor kelp alone will inspire them to feed, brine shrimp combined with the smell of kelp provides the threshold stimulus necessary to begin feeding. Rhinophore ablation indicates that the rhinophores are the chemoreceptive organs responsible for the detection of these cues. INTRODUCTION Chemoethology is the effect of chemical stimuli on behayjor. The two principal forms of chemoreception are olfaction and gustation. There are numerous ways of differentiating between the two: necessary concentrations for detection, solubility. volatility, organs for detection, and distance. In this paper. distance is the criterion used to distinguish between the two. Smell is distance chemoreception, while taste is touch chemoreception. Underwater, olfaction is one of the most useful senses. Highly volatile compounds such as amino acids, which on land can only be tasted, are water soluble. This allows them to impart important chemical messages in the water. Chemical stimuli are especjally useful because they occur in large quantities, can be detected in either light or dark, and can impart highly specific messages, despite needing simple receptors. (For review see Croll, 1983 and Ache, 1987) Chemoreception has been shown to play a role in initiating many behaviors in molluscs. Chemoreception has been shown to be important in homing (Gelperin, 1973), feeding (Frings and Frings, 1965), and localization of conspecifics (Audesirk and Audesirk, 1977). Much of the research done with chemoreception has used amino acids as stimuli. (Agersbourg, 1922; Bicker et al, 1982; Jahan¬ Parwar, 1972) However, for the purpose of this experiment, used stimuli that are found in the natural environment of the Melibe. This allowed for the targeting of behaviors which are triggered by natural stimuli. Although amino acids are the most probable source of smell, the ability of an animal to detect both the unique combination of amino acids and concentrations of amino acids in a specific stimulus provides more insight into the role of smell in the natural environment than would experiments employing isolated and or increased concentrations of amino acids. Behavioral studies were performed to determine which stimuli to study and to determine which behaviors can be¬ triggered by chemoreception. The rhinophore is the principal organ which has been indicated in chemoreception in numerous nudibranchs. In Melibe leonina, a rhinophore of between Amm and 6mm in length is located within a small pocket of tissue on the tip of each of the rhinophore bearing processes (rbp). (Figure 1) The rhinophore is highly convoluted; therefore, it has a large surface area. This allows for an increased area with which it can contact different water borne molecules. In an active animal the rhinophore is exposed; howeyer, when an animal is stressed it retracts the rhinophore into the pocket. The rhinophore is innervated by a nerve (C4) which leads to the tentacular lobe of the central ganglion. (Hurst, 1968). Unlike the other three cerebral nerves which innervate the oral hood, C4 has no interconnections with other cerebral nerves. This implies that information from the rhinophore is carried directly to the brain without integrating information from other systems. This paper seeks to define specific behaviors which are triggered by chemical cues, and to locate the anatomical basis for the detection of these cues. MATERIALS AND METHODS Collection and Maintenance Melibe leonina were collected from the kelp forest near Del Monte Beach in Monterey, California at depths between 10 and 35 feet. The Melibe were contained in tanks with an open flowing sea water system at 100-15°C. Macrocystis and a variety of snails were added to provide the Melibe with a natural and clean environment. Melibe were fed either mysids collected by dip net in the Hopkins Marine Life Refuge or commercially grown brine shrimp every other day. Chemical detection Melibe were placed in specially designed tanks to test chemoreceptive ability. (Figure 2) The tanks were 15-liter Plexiglass“ tanks. Plexiglass"" dividers were caulked in the center to divide the tanks into two compartments. Two holes of 6.3cm in diameter were drilled in the dividers, and Nytex screen was caulked over the holes to allow passage of water and chemical stimuli through the tank. Seawater between 110 and 15°C flowed freely into the compartment in which the stimulus was contained. Melibe were placed in the other compartment and were allowed to acclimate for at least six hours before a stimulus was added. The water drained from the tank through a standpipe in the same compartment as the Melibe. Only one animal was placed in each tank for each trial, and the tanks were washed with fresh water between trials. Baseline behavioral observations were taken with no stimuli to determine the behavior of the Melibe in a sterile environment. Macrocystis and mysids, the stimuli used, were chosen because they serve as the preferred location and food source, respectively. To study the chemoeffective properties of macrocystis, two fronds of macrocystis, each with a length of 55-65cm, were added to the stimulus compartment of the tank. For the mysid trials, approximately thirty mysids were added to the stimulus compartment. The animals were then observed for a 30 minute period. To control for variations in water flow and light due to the presence of the stimuli, trials were conducted in the same manner using macrocystis shapes made from, black plastic to control for the presence of macrocystis or brine shrimp to control for the presence of mysids. Anatomy The pouch containing the rhinophore was excised from a conscjous animal using irridectomy scissors. The animals were then tagged by injecting them with 1cc of either methyl blue or fast green dye. In controls a comparably sized piece of tissue was removed in the same manner from the area adjacent to the rhinophore on control animals. To guard against infection, all surgically altered animals were individually isolated for twenty-four hours in tanks which for ten minutes every two hours received an inflow of UV and heat sterilized seawater filtered to 5u. Trials were conducted in the same manner as for the normal animals. Stimuli studied were macrocystis, control macrocystis, and mysids. RESULTS The behaviors that were observed during Melibe observation are defined as follows: resting foot and hood resting on the substratum tentacles folded into the hood. Tentacles may or may not be visible. scanning: continuous head motion while body remains still. walking locomotion along the substratum. treading back and forth movement of the body while the foot is still attached to the substratum swimming: complete removal of foot from the substratum with consequent locomotion through water. open hood an extension of the hood beyond the point where tentacles are extended. The baseline behavioral data obtained, when the Melibe were observed for thirty minutes with no stimuli indicates a low level of activity. The only behavior that occurred was scanning. Three of the ten Melibe tested performed this behavior. The remaining seven Melibe remained in the resting position for the entire test period. (Table 1) Response to Macrocystis When normal Melibe were exposed to macrocystis, all 13 animals tested came out of the resting state. Behaviors included scanning (11/13), walking (12/13), treading (11/13), swimming (7/13), and hood opening 7713. (Table 2) All the animals tested performed one of the locomotive behaviors: walking, treading, or swimming. When the simulated macrocystis was used as the stimulus the only behavior observed was scanning. This behavior was seen in six of nineteen animals tested. The remaining animals stayed in the resting state. (Table 2) Äfter rhinophore ablation, fewer animals came out of the resting state in response to macrocystis stimulus. (Table 3) Both the scanning and open hood behaviors were observed (2710), (1710). There was no appreciable difference between the behavior of the normal animals and those which had a small piece of rbp removed. Response to Mysids When normal Melibe were exposed to mysid thirteen of thirteen animals came out of the resting state. The oberved behaviors were scanning (12/13), walking (4/13), treading (2/13), amd swimming (1/13). (Table 4) The only behavior observed when normal Melibe were exposed to the smell of brine shrimp was scanning (3/13). (Table 4) Furthermore, when Melibe were exposed directly to brine shrimp, only two of the thirteen began to feed. However, under these conditions, introduction of macrocystis into the stimulus compartment of the tank initiated feeding in all but one of the animals. (Figure 3) 0 Animals with their rhinophores removed did not respond to the presence of mysid. In the ten trials, only one animal came out of the resting state. This animal both scanned and opened its hood (Table 3) DISCUSSION The major finding of this study is that Melibe leonina perform specific behaviors as a result of olfactory information detected by the rhinophore organ. The data indicate that Melibe alter their behavior based on chemical cues from their environment. Introduction of macrocystis in the stimulus compartment causes a transition from a resting state into a motile state. Introduction of mysid causes a transition from a resting state into a feeding state. The indication that the cue is olfactory, as opposed to visual or tactile, is that the behavioral transition is not accomplished with plastic macrocystis shapes or with brine shrimp alone. (Figures 4 and 5) However, macrocystis and brine shrimp presented together are sufficient to evoke feeding behavior. These data indicate that the Melibe world is olfactory as well as tactile. Macrocystis evokes locomotion indicating that chemical stimuli appears to trigger behavior that is useful in the natural environment of the Melibe. Macrocystis is the location of a number of important behaviors: egg laying, food finding, and location of conspecifics. Macrocystis has a characteristic secretion, providing Melibe with an olfactory stimulus underwater. Although Melibe are sensitive to changes in light and in water flow, experiments with simulated macrocystis indicate that the chemical attributes of the macrocystis, not other factors, trigger an increase in activity. This ability is important because it increases the probability that the Melibe relocates the macrocystis if it is detached. However, when Melibe are attached to macrocystis, they move infrequently. A possible explanation for this is that there is a tactile inhibition by macrocystis which overides the olfactory input. Another explanation is that the animals habituate to the smell of the macrocystis. Mysids also evoke heightened activity in the Melibe. In contrast to macrocystis, this chemical stimulus triggers hood opening. This serves two purposes. It allows for an increased surface area for tactile stimulation. Furthermore, once they receive the tactile stimulus, all the melibe must do is close their mouths around their prey. The nature of the behavior makes the chemical trigger essential. Because of water currents, a tactile stimulus is probably less useful under water. More importantly, Melibe move relatively slowly. If they were to wait for a tactile stimulus to begin feeding, their prey would escape before they could catch it. The experiments with brine shrimp indicate that changes in water flow alone can not account for the hood opening behavior in response to mysid. Furthermore, the experiments allowing tactile stimulation by brine shrimp indicate that tactile stimulation to a resting animal is not enough to trigger feeding in a sterile environment. However, when macrocystis smell is introduced into the system, feeding behavior is initiated. One explanation of the integration of these two stimuli to yield feeding is that the concomitant relaxation of the hood to allow stimulation of the tentacles initiates the feeding behavior. Although the mechanism for this phenomena is not understood at present, this experiment does indicate that Melibe are capable of integrating chemical cues. The rhinophore ablation experiments indicate that the rhinophores are the principal chemosensory organs for the passage of afferent information regarding the smell of macrocystis and mysid. There is a negligible difference in the responses of the rhinophorectomized animals exposed to chemoeffective stimuli, those exposed to control stimuli, and normal animals exposed to control stimuli. (Figure 6) This contrasts with the Pleurobranchaea californica in which it has been shown that the rhinophores, tentacles and oral veil are all receptive to the same chemical stimuli. (Bicker et al, 1982) while there is no indication of a lack of sensitivity of other organs of the Melibe to macrocystis and mysid, the removal of the rhinophores yields a decrease in behavioral responses to the comparable levels of an unstimulated control animal. This indicates that the rhinophore may be the sole organ responsible for the detection of these stimuli. Rhinophores may potentially detect many other stimuli besides mysids and macrocystis. Furthermore, they may be sensitive to variations in quantities of these stimuli. The independence of C4 may be indicative of the complexity of the rhinophore. he interconnecting pathways of the other cerebral neurons allows for reflex actions and short circuits without involving the central ganglion. (Hurst 1968). In contrast, nerve impulses from C4 must form synapses on the central ganglia. This may be necessary for the decoding of olfactory information from the rhinophore, and subsequent translation into evoked behavjors. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 1 would like to thank my advisor, Stuart Thompson, for his help and enthusiasm. I would also like to thank Mary Lucero for sharing her vast knowledge (and reprints) on olfaction. I am grateful to Karla Palmeri for the use of the tanks, space, and for answering all my questions about animal care, Chris Patton for his help with photography, and John Lee for all his mechanical help and an endless supply of caulk. Chris Mathes deserves thanks for a great deal of encouragement and for forcing my to put things in perspective. Thanks to Sam Wang for always taking the time to discuss science, (Sam..., do you have a minute?") and for vast amounts of constructive criticism. Thanks to everyone at Hopkins for being helpful and friendly. I'd like to express my gratitude to the spring class, especially to Agnieszka Rawa for always asking questions, William Nathaniel Timmins for providing cookies and the answer to the ultimate question about gustation of Melibe, and Amanda Schivell for her boundless enthusiasm and open ears during the Melibe and mysid hunts. Literature Cited Ache, Barry W. 1987. Chemoreception in Invertebrates. Pp. 39¬ 64. In: T.E. Finger & W.L. Silver (eds.), Neurobiology of Taste and Smell. John Wiley & Sons, Inc: New York. Agersborg, H.P.K. 1921. Some observations on qualitative chemical and physical stimulations in nudibranchiate mollusks with special reference to the role of the 'rhinophores. 1921. Journal of Experimental Zoology 36: 423-444. Audesirk, T.E. & G.J. Audesirk. 1977. Chemoreception in Aplysia californica. II. Electrophysiological evidence of detection of the odor of conspecifics. Comparitive Biochemistry and Physiology 564:267-270. Bicker Gerd, W.J. Davis and EM. Matera. 1982. Chemoreception and mechanoreception in the gastropod mollusc Pleurobranchia californica. Journal of Comparitive Physiology 149:221-234. Croll, Roger P. 1983. Gastropod Chemoreception. Biological Review 58:293-319. Frings, Hubert and Carl Frings. 1965. Chemosensory bases of food-finding and feeding in Aplysia juliana (Mollusca, Opistobranchia). Biological Bulletin 128:211-217. Gelperin, Alan. 1974. Olfactory basis of homing behavior in the giant garden slug, Limax maximus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA) 71:66-70. Hurst, Anne. 1968. The Feeding Mechanism and Behaviour of the Opistobranch Melibe leonina. Symposium of the Zoological 0. Society of London zz:151-166. Jahan-Pawar, Behrus. 1972. Behavioral and electrophysiological studies on chemoreception in Aplysia. American Zoologist 2:525-537. FIGURE LEGEND 1. Dorsal view of Melibe leonina. 2. Sketch of chemical detection tanks. 3. Tank set-up for brine shrimp experiment. Animals observed for thirty minutes after introduction of the stimulant. a. brine shrimp in stimulus compartment evokes no response from melibe b. brine shrimp in Melibe compartment evokes no reponse from melibe c. kelp in stimulus compartment and brine shrimp in Melibe compartment evokes a feeding response. 4. Comparison of the percentage of Melibe tested performing specific behaviors in response to smell of kelp and control kelp within 30-minute period. The behavioral totals may exceed 100% because individual Melibe may perform more than one behavior during the observation period. At least 50% of the animals performed each behavior in response to kelp. In response to the control, only 328 of the animals were responsive and only one behavior was performed. 5. Comparison of the percentage of Melibe tested performing specific behaviors in response to smell of mysid and brine shrimp within a 30-minute period. All tested animals opened their hoods in response to mysid, and many animals exhibited other behaviors as well. In contrast, none of the Melibe opened their hoods in response to brine shrimp smell and only 25% of tested animals came out of the resting state. 6. Comparison of rhinophorectomized Melibe exposed to chemoeffective stimuli and normal Melibe exposed to control stimuli during a thirty minute period. Rhinophorectomized animals exposed to chemoeffective stimuli display similar levels of behavior to normal animals exposed to control stimuli. Table Legend 1. Observed behaviors of Melibe when no stimulus was introduced into the tanks. Each row represents the behavior of an individual animal during the thirty minute observational period. 2. Observed behaviors of Melibe during a thirty minute period after macrocystis or plastic was introduced into the stimulus compartment of the tanks. Each row represents the behavior of an individual animal during the observational period. In response to kelp all animals performed at least one of the locomotive behaviors (i.e. walking, treading, and swimming) 3. Observed behavior of rhinophorectomized Melibe in response to plastic, macrocystis, or mysid. Each row represents the behavior of an individual animal during the observational period. 4 Observed behaviors of Melibe in response to mysid or brine shrimp. Each row represents the behavior of an individual animal during the observational period. O S C RHINOPHORE RHINOPHORE BEARING PROCESS CERATA STIMULUS -INFLOV STANDPI O Soon Pong Lrnd 43 +5 S RESPONSES TO MACROCYSTIS OR CONTROL DURING 30 MINUTE INTERVAL NORMAL ANIMALS MACROCYSTIS (N=13) 100% E CONTROL (N=19) 80% 40% 20% 0% SCAN WALK TREAD SWIM OENT BEHAVIOR 100% 80% 60% 40% 20% RESPONSES TO MYSID OR BRINE SHRIMP DURING 30 MINUTE INTERVAL NORMAL ANIMALS SMYSID (N=13) □ BRINE SHRIMP (N=12) 8 1 SCAN WALK TREAD SWIM ONHOOO BEHAVIOR NORMAL ANIMALS' RESPONSES TO CONTROL STIMULI DURING 30 MINUTE INTERVAL 100% Q CONTROL MACROCYSTIS (N=19) E NO STIMULUS (N=10) 80% □ BRINE SHRIMP (N=12) 60% 40% 20% a a a ooo o08 tataa- 09 A WALK TREAD SWIM OPENHOO SCAN BEHAVIOR RESPONSES TO RHINOPHORECTOMIZED ANIMALS SIACROCYSTIS OR CONTROL DURING 30 MINUTE INTERVAL 100% MACROCYSTIS (N=10) □ CONTROL (N=13) 80% 60% 40% 20% a akataa- 0% WALK TREAD SCAN SWIM ONNHO BEHAVIOR RHINOPHORECTOMIZED ANIMALS' RESPONSES TO MYSID DURING 30 MINUTE INTERVAL 100% E MYSID (N=10) 80% 60% 40% 20% 0% TREAD SWIM WALK OPENHOOD SCAN BEHAVIOR STIMULUS NO STIMULUS NO STIMULUS NO STIMULUS NO STIMULUS NO STIMULUS NO STIMULU NO STIMULUS NO STIMULUS NO STIMULUS NO STIMULUS SCAN WALK TREAD SWIM OPENHOOD STIMULUS CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL MACROCYSTIS MACROCY. MACROCYSTIS MACROCYS MACROCY MACROCY: MACROCYSTIS MACROCYSTIS MACROCY. MACROCYST MACROCY MACROCYSTIS MACROCYST SCAN WALK TREAD SWIM OPENHOOD STIMULUS BRINE SHRIMP BRINE SHRIMP BRINE SHRIMP BRINE SHRIMP BRINE SHRIMP BRINE SHRIMP BRINE SHRIMP BRINE SHRIMP BRINE SHRIMF BRINE SHRIMP BRINE SHRIMP BRINE SHRIMP MYSID MYSID MVS MYSID MYSID MYSID MYSID MVS MVS MYSID MYSID MYSID MYSID SCAN WALK TREAD SWIM OPENHOOD STIMULUS CONTROL OONTROL CONTRO CONTROL CONTRO CONTROL CONTRO NTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL CONTROL MACROCYSTIS + MACROCYSTIS + MACROC MACROCYSTIS - MACROCYSTIS STIS- MACROCY MACROCYSTIS- MACROCYSTIS - MACROCYSTIS MACROCYSTIS - MV. MYSID MYSID MYSID MYSID MYSIC MYSID MYSID MYSID MYSID SCAN WALK TREAD SWIM OPNHOOD