ABSTRACT

The keyhole limpet Fissurella volcano has a sequential escape response
to stimulation by tube feet of the predatory starfish Pisaster ochraceus.
The response consists of 1) elevation of the shell at the point of
stimulation, 2) extrusion of the mantle over the shell, 3) rotation of the

shell around the keyhole axis, and 4) rapid locomotion away (fleeing).
Limpets on horizontal surfaces have a stronger response to tube feet than do
those on vertical surfaces. Responses to sea water which presumably
contains a water soluble starfish scent consist of only the mantle extrusion
response. Finally, receptors mediating detection of the starfish substance
appear to be located on the tips of the mantle tentacles.
INTRODUCTION
A variety of gastropods have adaptive avoidance and escape reactions to
predatory starfish (Bullock, 1953) and react to their predators both upon
contact and at a distance. Detection of the predator is thought to involve
chemoreception. A substance which may be involved in detection was isolated
from Pisaster ochraceus and is most likely attached to proteins within the
tube feet (Feder and Lasker, 1964).
Numerous limpets respond to starfish. The Collisella and Acmaea
limpets have a stereotyped response to tube foot contact: raising the shell,
rocking from side to side, rotation, and fleeing (Bullock,1953). Other
limpets such as Fissurella volcano's relative Diodora aspera exhibit a much
different response. This response consists of extruding mantle tissue from
the outer margin of the shell and out the keyhole to prevent starfish tube
feet from fastening to the limpet shell (Margolin, 1964).
The present study characterizes the response of Fissurella volcano to
contact with Pisaster ochraceus tube feet and to water which had been
exposed to a living starfish. Differences in the escape response in limpets
tested on horizontal vs. vertical surfaces were also studied. In addition,
morphological investigations of the suspected sensory elements were
undertaken.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Specimens were collected at The Great Tide Pool in Pacific Grove,
California during low tides. They were segregated into two groups depending
on the orientation of the surface on which they were found (ie. horizontal
and vertical). Limpets were placed in tanks and maintained on surfaces in
their respective orientations.
For scanning electron microscopy the limpet was first stimulated using


a tube foot to increase tentacle tip exposure and then immediately submerged
in low calcium sea water containing 460mM Nacl, lmM Cacl, 50mM MgCl, 1OmM

KCl, 10mM Hepes, and 5mM tetracaine, a solution which prevents contraction



of the mantle. The limpet was subsequently de-shelled and fixed in a

solution containing 12 glutaraldehyde, 12 para-formaldehyde, 0.22 sodium

cacodylate, and 852 sea water at pH 7.3. Pieces of the mantle tissue with


tentacles were then dissected and dehydrated in a graded acetone series.

The remaining liquid was removed by critical point drying.

Behavioral analysis was carried out using a video camera and tape
recorder. A magnifying glass placed on the camera lens increased

magnification of the subject. Limpets were tested on glass surfaces where

they were manually stimulated by a tube foot held by forceps. These
surfaces were either horizontal or vertical. A code that characterized the
four components of the behavior was devised:
L = lifting of the shell, generally at the point of stimulation
M = extruding the mantle over the shell
R = rotation around the keyhole axis
F = fleeing
L,M, and R were each preceded by a descriptive symbol. L was accompanied by
one of the following descriptions to indicate where the shell lifted up:
L = left
R = right
B = back
F = front
M was rated on a scale of O to 3 where O was no mantle response, 1 referred
to the first row of tentacles sticking out slightly from below the shell, 2
implied that the mantle extended over the shell but the full response was
not yet reached, and 3 indicated that the full response took place.
Finally, R was preceded by the magnitude of the angle turned which was found
by measuring along the longitudinal axis. Above this number an arrow was
written indicating the direction turned. Using this code the behavior could
be characterized more consistently and accurately. Timing of the behavior
was done using a stopwatch and taking an average time from the videotapes.


Reaction time of the timer was subtracted for calculations.
Detection of the starfish at a distance was tested by first placing a
starfish into an elevated bath of running sea water. Rubber tubing was
connected to the container and carried the "starfish water" over a ramp and
to the limpets. Limpets were placed in a trough with no ends so that water
would flow over the limpets unidirectionally at 0.165m/s. The limpets were
measured and placed 120mm from the downstream end of the container in
alternating directions and subsequently observed for 30 minutes.
RESULTS
Anatomy of the Mantle Tentacle System
Figure 1 illustrates the gross morphology of the mantle tentacles of
Fissurella volcano. There are two parallel bands of tentacles. All of the
tentacles are highly vascularized by blood sinuses and can be inflated by
the pumping of blood into them from the very large heart. A similar single
band of tentacles encircles the keyhole region. The picture depicted in
Figure 1 is that of the animal after it has displayed the mantle response to
starfish tube feet stimulation. The most peripheral band of tentacles
extends upwards and over the top of the shell. The second group of
tentacles protrudes from beneath the shell and lies parallel to the
substrate to which the limpet is attached. The tentacles are irregularly
and multiply branched.
More detailed morphology of the tentacle tips as revealed by scanning
electron microscopy is shown in Figure 2. Clusters of ciliary structures,
several microns in diameter, are located on the tentacles and seem to be
most common on the tips of the tentacles (Figure 24). Examination of a
cluster of ciliary structures at higher magnification (Figure 2B) shows that
each cilium is capped by a round bulb. These structures thus appear to be
"paddle cilia," which are thought to be chemosensitive in many molluscan
tissues (Haszprunar, 1985). Each cluster is seated in a small pit in the
microvillar coating of the tentacle epithelium.
Upon stimulation of various parts of the mantle and foot with a tube
foot, the response was elicited most strongly when touched on the tips of
the tentacles. From this information in conjunction with the scanning
electron micrographs, it appears that the ciliary structures are probably
the chemoreceptors involved in starfish detection. These receptors in
Fissurella are arranged very differently from those of Notoacmea scutum, a
limpet which runs from Pisaster. Unlike Fissurella, the receptors are much
more highly concentrated on the tips of Notoacmea tentacles and are not
embedded in pits in the epithelium covering the mantle (Phillips, 1977).
Response of Fissurella to Predatory Starfish
The entire escape sequence was only elicited upon stimulation of the
tentacles with starfish tube feet. Mechanical stimulation of the tentacles
resulted in withdrawal of the mantle.
The general sequence of the response was derived from the coded videos
and is schematized in Figure 3. It consisted of:
1) Lifting (Figure 3B)
2) Mantle extrusion (Figure 30)
3) Rotation (Figure 3D)
4) Fleeing (not illustrated)
Lifting involved the upward tilting of the shell at the point of
stimulation. This resulted in a broad exposure of the foot to the starfish.
Approximately half the surface area of the shell was subsequently covered by
mantle tissue emanating from both out of the apical orifice (ie. keyhole)

and upwards around the rim of the shell.
L and M occur is every limpet stimulated. Although R and F components
occurred with less frequency than L and M, they too are important elements
of the behavior. Rotation generally turned the limpet away from the tube
foot stimulus (See also below). Fleeing served to move the limpet still
farther away.
Limpets placed in a flow of water which had run over a starfish
upstream also invariably display the mantle response. The time scale in
which they react is on the order of minutes (Figure 4). The reaction occurs
between two and twelve minutes - a much longer time frame than that required
for response to tube foot stimulation (See also below). Control animals
exposed to an identical flow of sea water which has not flowed over a
starfish do not exhibit this behavior. Neither the experimental group nor
the control group lift, rotate, or flee.
Observations of living starfish in the presence of Fissurella indicate
that the limpet's mantle response stimulates an avoidance reaction by the
starfish. Pisaster's tube feet appear to be repelled by the mantle tissue
upon contact. However, they will grab on to any exposed portion of the
shell.
Time Course of Mantle Extrusion Response
Although all limpets reacted rapidly to contact with a starfish tube
foot, the size of the limpet influenced the time it took to begin the
extrusion of the mantle following stimulation. Figure 5 indicates that as
the length of the limpet increases, latency of the response also increases.
Mantle extrusion is not an instaneous process and shows a good degree

of variability in its time course. The total time required to complete the

mantle response was therefore measured (Figure 6). This time ranged from 1
to 15 seconds. The graph is, however, not adjusted for size of the limpet.

Direction and Degree of Rotation Dependence on Stimulus Site
Figure 7 illustrates results from a series of experiments indicating
that the point of stimulation determines the direction and the magnitude of
the angle that the limpet will rotate through. The central oval in the
figure represents a keyhole limpet (dorsal view) where the anterior end
corresponds to zero degrees. Each numbered line refers to the point of
stimulation measured as an angle with respect to the zero mark. The open
circles contain information referring to angles turned at each point of
stimulation. Arrows on the circles indicate the direction and magnitude of
the angle turned by an individual limpet relative to the zero point. The
dotted lines correspond to the means of the angles turned for clockwise and
counterclockwise rotation. A closed circle refers to an individual limpet
which did not detectably rotate. Detection sensitivity was 15'.
From this figure it is apparent that the limpet turns in such a way so
as to move its head away from the stimulation point especially at the 0, 45,
and 90° points. Stimulation at 0° evokes a strong counterclockwise
preference for rotation. Rotating seems less consistent and less marked
with 135 and 180° stimulation. As shown more quantitatively below, the
magnitude of the angle of rotation increases steadily as the limpet is
stimulated closer to its head.
Responses of Horizontal vs. Vertical Limpets
Differences in the intensity of each of the behavioral components of
limpets tested on horizontal vs. vertical surfaces were found. Analysis of
rotation by horizontally oriented limpets is continued in Figure 8 where the
mean absolute value (+ standard error of the mean) of the degree of rotation
for horizontal limpets is plotted vs. absolute value of stimulation site
(ie. direction of rotation is ignored). Magnitude of rotation decreases
smoothly as the point of stimulation approaches the posterior end of the
limpet.
The magnitude of rotation for vertically oriented limpets is smaller
than that of the horizontal limpets for all points of stimulation except at
135° where they are not detectably different. Limpets tested on a vertical
surface also exhibit a lesser degree of rotation as the stimulus site moves
posteriorly but may show a more abrupt decrease in angle size as stimulation
moves from 0° to 45° than do horizontal limpets. The smaller sample size of
vertical limpets makes such detailed comparisons difficult though.
Fleeing was also studied in horizontal vs. vertical limpets. As shown
in Figure 9A, whether or not a limpet on a horizontal surface will flee once
stimulated seems random. In comparison, vertical limpets flee far less
frequently. Scaling (x 41/27) of the vertical numbers plotted so they
represent the same sample size as the horizontal values does not alter this
conclusion. This difference in fleeing frequency between horizontal vs.
vertical limpets is highly significant by the Chi square test.
Magnitudes of the mantle extrusion response for limpets on horizontal
vs. vertical surfaces also differ dramatically. Figure 9B shows results on
an analysis in which mantle extrusion was estimated with the O - 3 scale
(See Methods) for each population. All limpets showed some response to tube
foot stimulation, but only 22 of horizontal vs. 452 of vertical limpets
stopped the response at the stage 2 level. Again, this difference is still
obvious when the vertical values are scaled (x47/35) to correspond to the
horizontal sample size (dotted line).
DISCUSSION
From this research it is evident that Fissurella volcano has a distinct
response to the starfish Pisaster ochraceus which undoubtedly serves as an
escape mechanism. Once provoked by the starfish, the limpet has a
stereotyped behavior which can be broken down into component parts: lifting,
extruding, rotating, and fleeing. Diodora aspera, a related keyhole limpet,
has a similar response to starfish, however, it only consists of two parts
(Margolin, 1964). These components include the mantle response and the
lifting of the entire shell as opposed to tilting specifically at the point
of stimulation as in Fissurella.
In investigating individual parts of the behavior, latency for the
mantle response was found to decrease as limpet size decreased. Why this is
true is not clear. The times involved are far longer than could be
accounted for by neuronal conduction, etc. Possibly the phenomenon is an
adaptive one. Because the tenacity of a limpet adhering to substrate must
depend on the surface area of the foot, smaller limpets may be much more
easily dislodged by a starfish than larger ones. Thus, it may be very
advantageous for small animals to react as quickly as possible to prevent
tube feet from attaching to their shells.
Another method that these limpets have evolved to avoid contact by
starfish is rotating the head away from the point of stimulation. Ability
to escape the starfish is thus enhanced by this directed turning by allowing
the limpet to flee by locomoting forward. Attack by a starfish closer to
the anterior end would require a larger degree of turning in order for the
limpet to face away from the intruder before fleeing. Precisely this
behavior was found experimentally - the closer stimulation is to the head,
the greater is the magnitude of the angle turned. When attacked at the
posterior end, the direction of rotation (if desirable at all) would not be
very critical to the survival of the limpet because the starfish is already
behind it. Again, this behavior was found experimentally - nearly all
limpets stimulated 180' opposite to the head fail to turn at all.
While the response to tube feet is fairly stereotypical, certain
differences do exist among limpets stimulated on surfaces with dissimilar
orientations (specifically, horizontal and vertical). Limpets on vertical
surfaces tend to have less intense responses than those probed on horizontal
surfaces. That is, vertical limpets flee less often, extrude their mantle
to a lesser degree more often, and nearly always display a smaller angle of
rotation than do horizontal limpets.
This apparent restraint in the vertical animals may again be adaptive.
Limpets on a vertical surface must have a far greater chance of being
dislodged by gravitational and water current effects if their ability to
grip is significantly decreased, as it must be when they are undergoing the
escape response. Falling exposes limpets to many serious dangers such as
different predators or not landing upright. Some degree of caution in
utilizing the escape response might be valuable for a vertical limpet.
Detecting a starfish at a distance is another important aspect of
Fissurella's behavioral repertoire. "Scent" detection allows the limpet to
prepare itself for a potential attack by extruding the mantle well before
contact occurs. The time for the response to begin is, however, much slower
than for reaction to a tube foot. The limpet, therefore, would not seem to
be constantly in a state of alarm and ready to react to a few water-borne

molecules. Similarly, the animal does not have to expend an unnecessary


amount of energy in maintaining the extruded mantle in a permanent defensive

position. The interplay between water-borne and contact-mediated behaviors
suggested here deserves additional study.

Unlike several other species of limpets, Fissurella volcano does not

flee when exposed to starfish water. Phillips found that Collisella

limatula and Notoacmea scutum move downstream when exposed to flowing water

which previously passed over a living starfish. In contrast, Fissurella

shows no obvious directed movement under identical conditions. Fissurella
only flees as a culmination of the entire escape response elicited by

contact between mantle tentacles and tube feet.


The mantle extrusion response probably more than compensates for this
deficiency in their behavior by repelling starfish tube feet. The fraction
of the shell that is exposed may not allow the starfish to get a good grip
on Fissurella. This combined with movement away from the starfish via
rotation and fleeing enhance its ability to escape predation. Despite much
superficial similarity in the responses to starfish of Fissurella,
Notoacmea, and Collisella limpets, definite differences exist in behavioral
details, receptor location and type, and in neural pathways involved. Such
specific differences would be expected based on the disparate taxonomic
positions of Fissurella and the other genera. Further investigation of
these differences may better explain the adaptive significance of the
limpets' respective responses in light of the particular environment in
which each lives.
FIGURE LEGENDS
Figure 1. Mantle tentacles. Mantle tentacles are depicted as they appear
following extrusion due to stimulation by a starfish tube foot.
Figure 2. Scanning electron microscopy of mantle tentacle. 2A) Possible
receptors on the tip of a tentacle are shown. Receptors are ciliary
structures. 2B) Ten times the above magnification of a receptor group sunk
into a pit in the coating of microvilli covering the tentacle tip.
Figure 3. Starfish escape response sequence. 3B) Limpet lifts its shell at
the point of stimulation, 30) extrudes its mantle, 3D) rotates, and then 3E)
flees.
Figure 4. Total number of limpets with a full mantle response increases with
time of exposure to starfish water or tube feet. The solid line depicts
those exposed to water previously run over a starfish. The dotted line
refers to the time course involved when touched with a tube foot.
Figure 5. Length vs. Time to begin mantle response. As the size of the
limpet increases, the time required to begin the mantle response increases.
Figure 6. Time vs. Number of limpets that have completed the mantle
response. This graph does not account for size of the limpets sampled. All
limpets eventually complete the response.
Figure 7. Direction and magnitude of angle rotated following tube foot
stimulation. See text for explanation.
Figure 8. Point of stimulation vs. degrees turned in horizontal and vertical


limpets. Differences between horizontal and vertical limpets are significant
(f992) at each angle except at 90° where they are marginally significant and
at 135°.

Figure 9. Differences exist in the escape responses of horizontal vs.

vertical limpets. Dotted lines refer to values adjusted for equal sample

sizes. 94) Representation of whether animals flee or not when tested on

horizontal vs. vertical surfaces. 9B) Degree of mantle extrusion response

vs. Number of limpets. The degree of response was measured on a scale of

zero to three where three represents the full mantle response.
BIBLIOGRAPIY

Bullock, T.H. (1953). Predator recognition and escape responses of some



intertidal gastropods in presence of starfish. Behaviour 5, 130-140.


Feder, H.M. and Lasker, R. (1964). Partial purification of a substance from


starfish tube feet which elicits escape responses in gastropod molluscs.


Life Sciences 3, 1047-1051.

Haszprunar, G. (1985). The fine morphology of the osphradial sense organs of

the mollusca. I. Gastropoda, Prosobranchia. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B.
307, 457-496.

Margolin, A.S. (1964). The mantle response of Diodora aspera. Animal
Behaviour 12(1), 187-194.
Phillips, D.W. (1975). Distance chemoreception-triggered avoidance behavior
of the limpets Acmaea (Collisella) limatula and Acmaea (Notoacmea) scutum to
the predatory starfish Pisaster ochraceus. The Journal of Experimental
Zoology 191(2), 199-209.
Phillips, D.W. (1977). A scanning electron microscope study of sensory
tentacles on the mantle margin of the gastropod Acmaea (Notoacmaea) scutum.
The Veliger 19(3), 266-271.
16
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Many thanks to Dr. William Gilly for his tremendous help in completing
this project and for the great fishing trips which definitely were
highlights of the quarter. Thanks also to Chuck Baxter for invaluable advice
and to the rest of the Hopkins people for making this a great learning
experience.
L
a E
LI
2

O
o
O










S

—
------
2

)





(9





k ta
A










31

2




2










.








4


.
2-






S

2




5

L
—








- - -





















—














—
















——



















—










5





























+










——

.


t









N
—.—
—”








——















—












.



——


10














—
















---
10
SNOSSSA JILNVN Tn HIIM aSaNON


-
.



—



















-- . -















——








——












——
———





























—
—.——








--

















3























——












—


u
—




—


—




2 1.6










9















E




1.2
—




— —







—-

O.8


—.

*


â

—.








——
—






—

-
O.4
- .



—










.




- -



L

2



9
0


——







LENGTH (mm)










—


—




— ——






——









--- --------—
1










—


:—

—










—-










—







----






























—




—







0










2


















U











—


...--

—
——.—





1







—












—-


L









—















—



——






—







—


+-



d








SLAdMIT 4O HaNON








—











—
—

—

1





















!



-

—







0
—
-

—
0
—














——+

—



















7


V
S
——
2

—
—




—




4










—


+)-
8
+
S





—

S
+
—

S




S
45
5

1—
+






—
—0




—
0




40



2




—
—
—/

—


—
1

90
90


0

—



—














X

—




——


—
—




4


9




4
— —
—





1

1—7
N

——

180


7




—


1
- —

-



—
S
+—. +
8
— —


—/—

—6






—








—
—















—










-









4





——














—






— —












——









—
80
—




















—0
4


20

1
1
— —

—0
30

—14
o







—
-0


+
—
10




—

—














——

—


.


—





-+

.. —
































—

+





3

HORIZONTAE






VERTICALE



















+






S

S



S
S

8







—


+



S






—




—




—




S



—






8


—

— —


—
—

—

L
45
90.—— 3
80
—


ka-


POINI




OTIMULATION



+
—

...


E.








—
. ----
——

—-—
—



HORIZONTAL






— —



..-—



———



VERTICAL


40

370
6
*


—











0


— ——
0.3
——

2

.

—








2

21




2

.



O


——
t



u

5


910


-1



—












——


—





77










e
0















































â
—.——




--














46


.... ..
..





S
. ..




40





6.2



.—
.












O



u








020




—
—



—

—

—-






—

10
-----
—

—


——.—
—
——


:


—: -
----
2

——.—
O
—
3

2











-


---.—
DEGREE OF
SPONSE
..


———

——


—.——





-—-
-

..



—




—— ——